Dersleri yüzünden oldukça stresli bir ruh haline sikiş hikayeleri bürünüp özel matematik dersinden önce rahatlayabilmek için amatör pornolar kendisini yatak odasına kapatan genç adam telefonundan porno resimleri açtığı porno filmini keyifle seyir ederek yatağını mobil porno okşar ruh dinlendirici olduğunu iddia ettikleri özel sex resim bir masaj salonunda çalışan genç masör hem sağlık hem de huzur sikiş için gelip masaj yaptıracak olan kadını gördüğünde porn nutku tutulur tüm gün boyu seksi lezbiyenleri sikiş dikizleyerek onları en savunmasız anlarında fotoğraflayan azılı erkek lavaboya geçerek fotoğraflara bakıp koca yarağını keyifle okşamaya başlar

GET THE APP

Developmental Trend of the Effect of Disgust Emotion on Moral Judgement in Children and Adolescents
ISSN: 2375-4494
Journal of Child and Adolescent Behavior
Make the best use of Scientific Research and information from our 700+ peer reviewed, Open Access Journals that operates with the help of 50,000+ Editorial Board Members and esteemed reviewers and 1000+ Scientific associations in Medical, Clinical, Pharmaceutical, Engineering, Technology and Management Fields.
  • Research Article   
  • J Child Adolesc Behav 2016, Vol 4(5): 318
  • DOI: 10.4172/2375-4494.1000318

Developmental Trend of the Effect of Disgust Emotion on Moral Judgement in Children and Adolescents

Ming Peng1, Lei Chang2,3* and Chi-shing Tse4
1Department of Psychology, Central China Normal University, Hong Kong
2Department of Psychology, University of Macau, MACAU, Hong Kong
3Department of Educational Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
4Department of Educational Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
*Corresponding Author: Lei Chang, City University of Macau, MACAU, Hong Kong, Tel: +853-8822-8386, Email: Chang@umac.mo

Received: 21-Oct-2016 / Accepted Date: 26-Oct-2016 / Published Date: 31-Oct-2016 DOI: 10.4172/2375-4494.1000318

Abstract

Disgusting emotion was evolved to avoid disease infection. When the risk of contracting disease increases, people become more compliant with social rules to avoid the infection of disease. Previous studies showed that when primed with disgusting emotion (vs. neutral emotion), young adults rated moral violation behaviors more severely. In the present study, we examined the developmental trend of the influence of primed disgusting emotion on moral judgment across age groups. Participants rated either disgusting or neutral pictures and then completed a standardized moral questionnaire. Results showed that 10 and 16-year-old participants’, but not 6 or 13-year-old participants, rated the moral violations more severely when disgust primed. These findings suggest that the influence of disgusting emotion on moral judgements is not constant but varies at different developmental stages.

Keywords: Disgust, Emotion, Moral judgment, Priming, Disease avoidance

Introduction

Disgusting emotion, as part of the disease avoidance system, not only guides our body but also guides our heart. Previous studies that investigated the relationship between disgust and moral judgment showed that, for example, experimentally evoked physical disgust render more stringent judgement on moral violations in adults [1-3]. In a typical paradigm, the ways to evoke physical disgust are diverse, such as posthypnotic suggestion, viewing disgusting video clips or photos, and exposure to an unpleasant odor. After being primed with disgusting emotion, participants are presented with short vignettes about moral transgressions, which can be related to physical disgust (e.g., having sex between cousins, eating a dead dog) or pure moral disgust (e.g., stealing, lying). In these studies, moral judgement is defined as the consequence of appraisal [4]. Participants are asked to judge how wrong they consider the behavior to be, the extent to which they would punish the actor [5], and how much they would like to avoid the act or actor [6]. The typical finding is that experimentally evoked physical disgust can bias participants’ moral judgements by rendering them more severe.

Most of these studies that investigated the relationship between disgust and morality involved adult participants. The disgust system responds to parasite pressure not just over evolutionary time, but over lifetimes [7]. Rozin, et al. [8] proposed a developmental model of disgust, which suggests that different categories of disgust elicitors become salient in certain sequence during lifespan development. Distaste emerges first and is then preceded by core disgust after which emerge animal reminder disgusts, followed by interpersonal disgusts (e.g., avoiding an ill or immoral person), and, finally, socio-moral disgusts. As disgusting emotion is acquired during one’s growing up, the association between disgust and morality may be developed when one gets older.

However, to our knowledge only one study examined the association between disgust and morality from a developmental perspective [9]. Peng and Chang showed that 10- and 21-year-old participants judged the moral violation behaviors more wrong in the disgusting condition than in the control condition, and 10-year-old participants rated higher avoidance scores in the disgusting condition than in the control condition. However, 7-year-old participants did not show any difference in the judgement of moral violations as indicated by wrongness scores, punishment scores, and avoidance scores. These results suggest a developmental trend in the influence of disgusting emotion on moral judgements. However, the interpretation of these results might be clouded by some methodological limitations in their study. First, unlike previous studies [10], Peng and Chang [9] did not use the neutral emotion prime in their control group, such that participants in their control group were not necessarily in the neutral emotion state. Second, Peng and Chang adopted Stevenson et al. [11] procedure and had participants rate the disgusting pictures on a likedislike scale. However, the extent to which participants disliked a picture might not necessarily reflect how disgusting they felt towards the picture so that the variation of their participants’ moral judgements might not solely be attributed to their disgusting (vs. neutral) feelings being induced by the pictures. In previous research, participants were typically asked to rate specific emotion when comparing the emotion induced in different conditions [2]. Third, Peng and Chang did not use the same set of moral violation behaviors in different age groups, such that the effects of disgusting emotion on judgements of moral violation behaviors could not easily be compared across different age groups in their study. Moreover, the moral violation behaviors they used did not cover all aspects of moral domains.

In the current study, we used disgusting pictures and building pictures as the primes to induce in participants disgusting emotion and neutral emotion, respectively. The participants were asked to evaluate the pictures on a 3-point scale of disgust feeling and then complete a standardized moral questionnaire that measured their moral reasoning. This questionnaire (i.e., Sociomoral Reflection Measure- Short Form [SRM-SF], [12] was widely used in previous studies across 23 countries [13]. It contains 11 items that comprise the core of morality, such as honoring contracts, being faithful to truth, and upholding justice. Suitable to the current study, SRM-SF can be used in samples of children as young as 5 years old [12,14]. Because we were more interested in participants’ moral judgements rather than their moral appraisal, we had them rate the extent to which they agreed with the items without having to explain their reasoning.

To track down the development of the disgust-morality association from childhood to late adolescence, we recruited the 6-, 10-, 13-, and 16-year-olds as our participants. Children in ages of 5-9 are in the stage of heteronomous morality, characterized by a strict adherence to rules and obedience to authority [15]. Thus, even though 6-year-old children have adult-like disgust responses, we predicted that their moral judgements would not be influenced by disgusting emotion [11,16]. Children older than 10 are at the conventional moral development stage [17] and were reported to have acquired moralrelated disgusting emotion. Stevenson et al. [11] revealed that the evaluative ratings of sociomoral items was increasingly negative from the 2.5 to 10.1 years old and remained quite similar after the age of 10.1. Danovitch and Bloom [18] asked children whether a behavior was regarded as disgusting (Study 1 and 2) and whether a disgusting face was associated with the event in the story (Study 3). They found that 10-year-old children were more likely to label moral violation behaviors as disgusting than 6- or 8-year-old children. This suggests that children may acquire moral disgust at 10 years of age. Moreover, moral judgements in 10-year-old children develop from heteronomy to autonomy, characterized by following self-regulating principles [15], which state that interests, feelings, intentions, and values are involved in moral judgements, suggesting that moral judgement can be impacted by incidental emotion. Based on this theorizing, we predicted that only 10-year-old, but not 6-year-old, participants’ moral judgements would be influenced by disgusting emotion. Finally, for 13- year-old (early adolescence) and 16-year-old participants (late adolescence), previous studies showed that moral reasoning skills and moral self-regulation constantly reinforce each other throughout the adolescence [19]. Hence, we expected that disgusting emotion would continuously influence the moral judgements after 13 years of age.

Method

Participants: Four hundred and 27 (49.89% female) students from a large city in central China participated in the study. The participants represent four age groups – 6, 10, 13, and 16 years old. Equal numbers of participants were sampled from each of the four age groups and the two genders were evenly distributed across the age groups. About the same numbers of participants in each age group were randomly assigned to the neutral and disgusting emotion conditions. Table 1 contains the sampling information.

  Age Group
  6 10 13 16
All participants 6.26 (SD=0.39) 10.09 (SD=0.41) 13.10 (SD=0.53) 15.83 (SD=0.49)
Female participants 6.25 (SD=0.38) 10.07 (SD=0.37) 13.08 (SD=0.52) 15.73 (SD=0.55)
Male participants 6.27 (SD=0.39) 10.12 (SD=0.46) 13.11 (SD=0.54) 15.95 (SD=0.39)
Sample size 110 104 102 111
M:F 54:56 52:52 53:49 55:56

Table 1: Participants’ information.

Stimuli: Priming stimuli consisted of 10 images depicting pathogen (e.g., maggots, gory wounds) in the disgusting condition and 10 images depicting buildings in the neutral condition. Before the study, we asked 28 adults (13 female, age M=24.07, SD=4.59) to rate the priming stimuli in accord to their disgusting feeling in a 5-point Likert scale [1 (not at all) to 5 (very much)] (Table 2). The ratings were significantly higher for the images in the disgusting condition than those in the neutral condition, F (1,27)=425.30, p<0.001.

  Neutral pictures
M (SD)
Disgusting pictures
M (SD)
Evaluation in pilot study (range: 1-5) 1.14 (.05) 4.01 (.38)
6 years (range: 0-2) 0.47 (0.47) 1.33 (0.42)
10 years (range: 0-2) 0.26 (0.26) 1.52 (0.34)
13 years (range: 0-2) 0.23 (0.30) 1.22 (0.48)
16 years (range: 0-2) 0.19 (0.24) 1.23 (0.35)

Table 2: Emotion evaluation of priming pictures.

We used the Chinese version of SRM-SF, originally developed by Gibbs et al. [12], as the standardized moral questionnaire. This scale consists of 11 items that assess the extent to which a participant agrees with a statement (e.g., parents keeping promises to children, judges sending people who break the law to jail). It was demonstrated to be reliable and valid in measuring one’s level of sociomoral reasoning [20] and was utilized in Chinese population in previous studies [21,22].

Procedure: The study was conducted in a quiet computer room in school settings. The 6-year-old participants were tested in groups of 10, whereas participants in other age groups were tested in groups of 20. Each participant sat in front of an individual computer. An experimenter explained the processes and requirements of the tasks and maintained the order during the study. The distance between every two participants was between 50 cm and 1 m to avoid interference. After participants arrived in the computer room, they were asked to fill out the demographic information such as gender and date of birth. Then, experimenter introduced the requirements of the forthcoming tasks. After that, participants were presented with priming pictures one at a time in their own computer screens and asked to judge each of them in a 3-point scale on how disgusting they felt towards the picture [0 (not at all) to 2 (very strong)]. The participants then completed the Chinese version of SRM-SF on their own computers, in which they were asked to assess how much they agreed with the statements from 0 (not at all) to 10 (very much). Stimuli in all tasks stayed on the screen until participants responded. For 6-year-old participants, because of their limited reading abilities, an experimenter read aloud the requirements and all the items in the experiment. The same content presented in a large screen at the time when experimenter read aloud the items. After participants had completed one item, they raised their hands. After all participants raised their hands, the experimenter read the next item. In the SRM-SF task, participants were told that a higher score means that they agreed with the statement more strongly. The experimenter made sure that all participants understood the concept of scoring. Throughout the study, another experimenter sat at the back of the computer room to maintain order and to answer questions.

Results

Prime pictures evaluation

We excluded one participant from the analysis because the data were not captured or saved. A 2 (condition: disgusting or neutral) × 4 (age group: 6, 10, 13, or 16 years of age) ANOVA yields a significant main effect of priming conditions (F (1,418)=831.41, p<0.001,ηp2=0.67) and of age groups (F (3,418)=8.24, p<0.001,ηp2 2=0.06). The interaction between priming and age groups was also significant (F (3,418)=5.59, p=0.001, ηp2=0.04). The interaction effect was ordinal registering differences between disgust and neutral conditions across age groups in magnitude but not in directionality. The mean disgust ratings were higher for pictures in the disgusting condition than those in the neutral condition in participants of 6 years of age (F (1,107)=101.51, p<0.001), 10 years of age (F (1,102)=438.22, p<0.001), 13 years of age (F (1,102)=152.19, p<0.001), and 16 years of age (F (1,109)=316.80, p<0.001. The mean disgust rating of disgusting pictures in 10-year-old participants was significantly higher than the other three age groups [p<0.05], whereas other age-related differences did not approach significance (all ps>0.16) (Table 2). In all of the analyses, gender registered no significant differences.

SRM-SF: The higher score the participants obtained, the stricter moral judgements they made. The internal consistency reliability estimate of the SRM-SF is 0.62. A 2 (condition: neutral or disgusting)~ 4 (age group: 6, 10, 13, or 16 in years) ANOVA yielded a significant main effect of age group, F (3,419)=22.39, p<0.001, ηp2=0.14, and a significant interaction effect between condition and age group, F (3,419)=2.99, p=0.031, ηp2=0.02. Follow-up analyses showed that both 10- and 16-year-old participants reported higher scores in the disgusting condition compared to the neutral condition, F (1,102)=4.66, p=0.03, and F (1,109)=5.05, p=0.03, respectively, but this difference did not occur in 6- or 13-year-old participants, F (1,108)=1.80, p=0.18 and F (1,100)=0.03, p=0.86, respectively (Figure 1).

child-and-adolescent-behavior-disgusting

Figure 1: The SRM-SF score of neutral and disgusting condition in four age groups. Note: The higher the SRM-SF score was, the more severe participants’ moral judgement was Error bars indicate the standard errors of the means.

Discussion

In the current study, we investigated the impact of disgusting emotion on the moral judgement across age groups. The significant differences on SRM-SF scores between the neutral and disgusting emotion condition occurred in 10- and 16-year-old participants. A stronger agreement with the statements of the SRM-SF represents more rigorous moral standard. Therefore, 10- and 16-year-old participants reported more rigorous moral standard when they were primed with disgusting emotion as compared to the neutral emotion.

Similar to the result of the previous study [9], we found that the youngest participants (i.e., 7 years old in Peng and Chang and 6 years old in our study) did not make more severe moral judgement when primed by the disgusting condition as compared to the neutral emotion condition, suggesting that the influence of disgusting emotion on moral judgements was not established in these age groups. It is possible that 6-year-old participants did not acquire moral-related disgusting emotion. Sociomoral disgust, the last category that induces disgusting emotion, typically occurs after the interpersonal contamination category that is acquired at 5 to 7 years of age [8,23-25]. Hence, sociomoral disgust was not likely fully acquired in 6-year-old participants. However, it is also possible that children in 6 years of age are still in the moral heteronomy stage [15]. That is, they make moral judgements according to external requirements, such as complying with parents’ or teachers’ instructions and requirements. Their internal feeling did not influence the criterion of judgement. Hence, the impact of disgusting emotion on the moral judgement does not manifest among the 6 to 7 year-old children.

In contrast to the 6-year-old counterparts, 10-year-old participants yielded higher scores in SRM-SF (i.e., using stricter moral criteria) in the disgusting condition than in the neutral condition, suggesting that the impact of disgusting emotion on moral judgements occurred in this age group. This finding is consistent with those of previous studies [11,18]. In this age group, the concepts of contamination and interpersonal disgusting were fully acquired. Moreover, their moral judgement was transformed from heteronomy to autonomy [15], such that they made judgements based on their inferences and feelings. Hence, the influence of disgusting emotion appeared in 10-year-old participants.

Contrary to our expectation that the influence of disgusting emotion would manifest after 10 years of age, our 13-year-old participants did not show any influence of disgusting emotion on moral judgements. Given that these participants have acquired adult like disgusting emotion, the absence of the relationship between disgusting emotion and moral judgement might not be attributed to their not having acquired sociomoral disgusting emotion. Rather, this finding might be due to other developmental characteristics (such as socioemotional development). It is worth noting that the 13-year-old young adolescents have in general reached puberty. The developmental changes in the dopaminergic system take place at puberty [26,27]. The dopaminergic activity is important to affective and motivational regulation that influences the processing of social and emotional information [28]. Therefore, the changes likely shape the course of socioemotional development in adolescence. Previous studies reported that scores on sensation seeking, risk preference, and reward sensitivity all increased from age 10 until mid-adolescence and declined thereafter [28]. When people seek sensation and risk, they are less likely to comply with rules and laws [28]. Hence, our 13-year-old participants did not make moral judgements more severely in the disgusting condition than in the neutral condition. It is noteworthy that the current study is the first to show the absence of the relationship between disgusting emotion and moral judgement in this age group and therefore more evidences should be sought in future studies.

The likelihood of taking risks and seeking sensation increases from childhood to adolescence and declines from adolescence to adulthood [28,29]. In the mid or late adolescence, the coordination of cognition and affect is facilitated by the maturation of cognitive control systems, fully-grown connections across cortical areas, and well-developed connections between cortical and subcortical regions. This explains why our 16-year-old participants showed influence of disgusting emotion on the moral judgement. One implication of our findings is that moral education and socialization should take into consideration that some aspects of the moral development is unnecessarily linear among children and adolescents. This less than linear development of disgust related moral judgment in part also explains why some of the existing findings that are inconsistent with linear moral development theories [30].

Conclusion

We investigated the influence of disgusting emotion on the moral judgement in different age groups. Disgusting or neutral emotion was induced in each group, and then they were asked to complete a standardized moral questionnaire (SRM-SF). We found significant differences between neutral and disgusting condition on the SRM-SF judgement in 10- and 16-year-old participants, but not in 6- or 13- year-old participants. Consistent with our prediction, the influence of disgusting emotion on the moral judgements did not appear in 6 years old participants, but it appeared in 10-year-old and 16-year-old participants. Contrary to our expectation, there was no influence of disgusting emotion on the moral judgement in 13-year-old participants. Future studies should further examine the influence of disgusting emotion on moral judgement in adolescents.

References

  1. David B, Olatunji BO (2011) The effect of disgust conditioning and disgust sensitivity on appraisals of moral transgressions. Pers Indiv Diff 50: 1142-1146.
  2. Schnall S, Benton J, Harvey S (2008) With a clean conscience: Cleanliness reduces the severity of moral judgements. Psychol Sci 19: 1219-1222.
  3. Ugazio G, Lamm C, Singer T (2012) The role of emotions for moral judgements depends on the type of emotion and moral scenario. Emotion 12: 579-590.
  4. Chapman HA, Anderson AK (2013) Things rank and gross in nature: a review and synthesis of moral disgust. Psychol Bull 139: 300-327.
  5. Horberg EJ, Oveis C, Keltner D, Cohen AB (2009) Disgust and the moralization of purity. J Pers Social Psychol 97: 963-976.
  6. Peng M, Chang L, Zhou R (2013) Physiological and behavioral responses to strangers compared to friends as a source of disgust. Evol Human Behav 34: 94-98.
  7. Curtis V, de Barra M, Aunger R (2011) Disgust as an adaptive system for disease avoidance behavior. Philos Trans Royal Soc B: Biol Sci 366: 389-401.
  8. Rozin P, Haidt J, McCauley CR (2000) Disgust. In: Lewis M, Haviland JM,-Jones (eds.) Handbook of emotions. Guilford Press, New York.
  9. Peng M, Chang L (2016) How disgust affects moral judgement across age groups. J Psychol Sci 39: 1110-1115.
  10. Wu BP, Chang L (2012) The social impact of pathogen threat: How disease salience influences conformity. Pers Indiv Diff 53: 50-54.
  11. Stevenson RJ, Oaten MJ, Case TI, Repacholi BM, Wagland P (2010) Children’s response to adult disgust elicitors: Development and acquisition. Develop Psychol 46: 165-177.
  12. Gibbs JC, Basinger KS, Fuller D (1992) Moral maturity: Measuring the development of sociomoral reflection. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
  13. Gibbs JC, Basinger KS, Grime RL, Snarey JR (2007) Moral judgement development across cultures: Revisiting Kohlberg’s universality claims. Develop Rev 27: 443-500.
  14. Snarey J, Keljo K (1994) Revitalizing the meaning and measurement of moral development. Human Develop 37: 181-186.
  15. Piaget J (1997) The Moral Judgement of the Child. New York: The Free Press; (Original work published 1932).
  16. Siegal M, Fadda R, Overton PG (2011) Contamination sensitivity and the development of disease-avoidant behaviour. Philos Trans Royal Soc B: Biol Sci 366: 3427-3432.
  17. Kohlberg L (1984) The Psychology of Moral Development: The Nature and Validity of Moral Stages (Essays on Moral Development, Volume 2). Harper & Row.
  18. Danovitch J, Bloom P (2009) Children's extension of disgust to physical and moral events. Emotion 9: 107-112.
  19. Killen M, Smetana J (2006) Handbook of Moral Development. Laurence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah NJ.
  20. Stadler C, Sterzer P, Schmeck K, Krebs A, Kleinschmidt A, et al. (2007) Reduced anterior cingulate activation in aggressive children and adolescents during affective stimulation: Association with temperament traits. J Psychiat Res 41: 410-417.
  21. Kou Y (1997) The relationship between adolescent moral judgement development and family cohesion. Psychol Develop Educ 13: 46-50.
  22. Zhang JX (2012) The moral development law of the Dai youth and how the culture. Master thesis, Minzu University of China.
  23. Fallon AE, Rozin P, Pliner P (1984) The child's conception of food: The development of food rejections with special reference to disgust and contamination sensitivity. Child Develop 566-575.
  24. Rosen AB, Rozin P (1993) Now you see it, now you don't: The preschool child's conception of invisible particles in the context of dissolving. Develop Psychol 29: 300-311.
  25. Rozin P, Fallon A, Augustoni-Ziskind M (1985) The child's conception of food: The development of contamination sensitivity to "disgusting" substances. Develop Psychol 21: 1075-1079.
  26. Chambers R, Taylor J, Potenza M (2003) Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A critical period of addiction vulnerability. Am J Psychiatry 160: 1041-1052.
  27. Spear P (2000) The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci Biobehavioral Rev 24: 417-463.
  28. Steinberg L (2008) A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Develop Rev 28: 78-106.
  29. Steinberg L, Monahan KC (2007) Age differences in resistance to peer influence. Develop psychol 43: 1531.
  30. Cam Z, Seydoogullari S, Cavdar D, Cok F (2012) Classical and Contemporary Approaches for Moral Development. Educ Sci: Theo Pract 12: 1222-1225.

Citation: Peng M, Chang L, Tse CS (2016) Developmental Trend of the Effect of Disgust Emotion on Moral Judgement in Children and Adolescents. J Child Adolesc Behav 4: 318. Doi: 10.4172/2375-4494.1000318

Copyright: © 2016 Peng M, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Top